Babies hold an almost universal appeal, even for the grouchiest among us. There’s just something about their big shining eyes and button noses that draws us in and stirs our most tender feelings.
In short, babies are cute.
Resistance is futile.
This cuteness is so obvious that we take it for granted. But what exactly is it about a baby that prompts our “oohs” and “ahs”?
According to Jeffrey Kurland, associate professor of biological anthropology and human development, “we are inherently attracted to a specific set of characteristics, including large, symmetrical heads, large eyes, small mouths, and small noses.” But why do almost all humans find this particular set of features so appealing?
Kurland says the answer lies in evolution. According to Darwin, individuals of a given species will exhibit a new trait if that trait provides a survival advantage, boosting an organism’s chances of living to see sexual maturity and successfully reproduce. But what does evolutionary fitness have to do with being cute?
One of two basic processes may be at work, Kurland suggests. In the first, one of our distant female ancestors was born with a slightly different makeup than her forebears, perhaps the result of some random genetic mutation. This change in her genes caused her to prefer babies with the features we see as cute. She passed on this preference to future generations, and thereafter infants with cuteness attracted more attention and received more care than their less cute compatriots. The cuties were therefore more likely to survive and reproduce, and their offspring inherited both their cuteness trait and their preference for cuteness. “Because the two traits became linked in this way, they both increased in the population,” says Kurland.
The second evolutionary possibility is much like the first, except that here cuteness runs more than skin deep. In this scenario, too, Kurland explains, one of our foremothers was born with an arbitrary preference for cute babies—to her they just look better. And again, she gives more attention and care to her cute babies, who survive in greater numbers to pass on both their own cuteness genes and the genes for her preference, and this genetic combination gradually increases in the population.
In this case, however, the babies do not merely look better—they are better, from an evolutionary standpoint. According to Kurland, infants who displayed cute features may actually have been healthier, and as adults more sexually attractive. They would have been more likely to survive and reproduce. Cuteness, in this case, is an indicator of good genes.
Again, the mother’s preference trait and the infant’s cuteness trait reinforce each other. Over many generations, mothers keep selecting cuter babies, and in turn strengthening their preferences.
In both cases, Kurland stresses, cuteness and the preference for cuteness are passed on not just through female offspring, but through males as well. Fathers, uncles, and big brothers—as well as aunts and grandmas and family friends—will prefer infants who are as cute as they themselves were. “The two traits spread through the entire population.”
Leave a Reply
You must be logged in to post a comment.